What makes us who we are psychology

In 1990, Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr. and his colleagues (including esteemed twin researcher and PT blogger Nancy L. Segal) at the University of Minnesota published a striking finding: About 70% of the variance in IQ found in their particular sample of identical twins was found to be associated with genetic variation. Furthermore, identical twins reared apart were eerily similar to identical twins reared together on various measures of personality, occupational and leisure-time interests, and social attitudes.

Bouchard's study, along with many others, has painted a consistent picture: genes matter. The studies say nothing about how they matter, or which genes matter, but they show quite convincingly that they indeed do matter. Genes vary within any group of people (even among the inhabitants of middle-class, Western society!), and this variation contributes to variations in these people's behaviors. This finding should not be understated; it counters many a prevailing belief that we are born into this world as blank slates, completely at the mercy of the external environment. Because our psychological characteristics reflect the physical structures of our brains and because our genes contribute to those physical structures, there are unlikely to be any psychological characteristics that are completely unaffected by our DNA.

The authors of many twin studies have claimed that the environments experienced by twins (or any two siblings) do little to create differences in intelligence and personality. In fact, it turns out that genetic variation is correlated with variations in people's environments, a finding that some theorists have interpreted to mean that genes help to create the environments (see earlier post)! The idea here is that certain environments set off an appetite in the genes that nudges individuals to engage in certain experiences, and the environment then responds in a reciprocal fashion that reinforces an individual's nature.

To be sure, twin studies have received much criticism. Indeed, the proliferation of advanced statistical techniques (such as structural equation modeling) and the implementation of additional controls have allayed some of the concerns, but certainly not all.

Nevertheless, our point here isn't to rehash all of these criticisms. Instead, we are more concerned with how findings from twin studies are often misunderstood, misinterpreted, and blown out of proportion. Not just be the media, but even by serious scientists who get their work published.

To put things in perspective, I teamed up with the well-known developmental psychologist David S. Moore to list eight facts about genes, twin studies, and the heritability statistic that may come as a surprise to many people—even biologists! We hope these facts will help clarify past and future misunderstandings.

1. Genes, by themselves, can't determine anything

Twin studies partition the variance in nature and the variance in nurture. This allows researchers to determine whether differences in genes or differences in the environment in a particular population are associated with more of the differences in observed behavior.

In reality, all biological and psychological characteristics are constructed during development, when genes interact with local environmental factors that can be influenced by the broader environment. Therefore, gene-environment interactions are understood to drive the development of all of our characteristics. Naked DNA (or RNA) is simply not sufficient to produce psychological or biological traits.

  • What Is Personality?
  • Find counselling near me

Therefore, when it comes to understanding the development of a trait in a particular person, nature can never be separated from nurture. As Matt Ridley has put it: "[Genes] are devices for extracting information from the environment. Every minute, every second, the pattern of genes being expressed in your brain changes, often in direct or indirect response to events outside the body. Genes are the mechanisms of experience."

2. Parents matter, and will always matter

Somehow, the finding that the shared environment plays only a small role in creating personality differences in adults (see Judith Rich Harris's work for a good review) sometimes translates in the media as: parenting doesn't matter. This couldn't be farther from the truth.

Personality Essential Reads

Take the most essential element: a child needs to be raised in a family, almost any kind of family, to develop the ability to speak a language. Since every single person in twin studies checks that box (i.e., is raised in a family of some sort), this factor never varies and thus does not predict differences in ability to speak a language. But does this mean that the variable "has a family" doesn't matter in determining whether or not a person develops the ability to speak a language? Of course not! That's like saying that water has no influence on a fish's development because all fish live in water. Just because a variable doesn't vary doesn't mean it has no causal impact on a particular outcome.

The parenting factors that are statistically associated with differences between individuals should never be confused with the parenting factors that cause the development of a trait within an individual. Genes could "account for" 100% of the variability in a trait in a particular twin study, but this does not mean that environmental factors are therefore unimportant in the development of the trait; parents still matter and will always matter.

It turns out that parenting matters, just in a way different than originally assumed. Genes matter to the extent that they support parenting—because like any other behavior, parenting behaviors are influenced by the genes—and parents matter to the extent that they support the expression of genes.

3. Heritability depends entirely on context

None of the twins in Bouchard's study were reared in real poverty, raised by illiterate parents, or were mentally retarded. There is reason to believe that under more dire circumstances, the heritability of IQ would be significantly lower than that reported by Bouchard. After all, if everyone was raised in an identical environment, variations in their psychological characteristics couldn't possibly be accounted for by anything other than variations in their genes (since there would be no variations in their developmental environments); the more variation in environments that twins in twin studies are exposed to, the lower the heritabilities we should expect to find.

In one study, Eric Turkheimer and colleagues studied 320 pairs of 7-year-old twins who were raised in extreme poverty. Among the poorest, the shared environment accounted for most of the differences in IQ (60%), and the genes accounted for very little; consequently, in this study, the heritability of IQ was reported to be close to zero! Among the richest, however, the heritability of IQ approached what Bouchard found: variations in the genes accounted for most of the differences in IQ scores, and the shared environment accounted for very little of the variance. This study points to the fact that estimates of heritability depend on the sample that is studied, and the environment of that sample.

Turkheimer's study should also be a reminder that just because something is heritable doesn't mean it's immutable. Remember the Flynn effect (see earlier post)? That's a reminder just how much the environment matters, even after completely controlling for genes (by looking at IQ changes across generations).

This raises a deeper point: depending on what you hold constant, you can either show a genetic contribution or an environmental contribution. The point is: both are always contributing to the development of any trait, and context matters in which accounts for more of the differences in a trait.

4. The actual heritability value simply does not matter

The heritability of a trait can vary from 0.00 to 1.00 depending on the environments from which research participants are sampled. Because we know that genes play some role in the development of any trait, the precise heritability estimate doesn't matter in a practical sense.

Heritability depends on the amount of variability in the environmental factors that contribute to a trait. The problem is that our understanding of the factors that contribute to the development of human traits in general—and to IQ in particular—is currently so deficient that we typically do not know if the environmental factors important in the development of a particular trait are stable across testing situations, vary somewhat across those situations, or vary wildly across those situations.

Even if a population of individuals were to develop in a range of environments believed to be the same as that in which a particular study was conducted, the results of that study would not allow us to predict developmental outcomes in the new range of environments, because the environmental factors that the researchers originally focused on-and controlled for-might not be the relevant environmental factors at all.

Instead, the crucial environmental factors might remain unmeasured, and consequently, variability of those factors across the new range of environments could easily be very different than the variability of those factors across the environments sampled in the original study.

Of course, we could just aim to measure all of the environmental factors that might affect the development of a trait. But it is not at all obvious prior to developmental analysis which environmental factors might make important contributions to the development of specific traits, so that approach would leave us measuring a seriously unwieldy number of variables.

Because the development of behavioral/psychological characteristics can be influenced by experiential factors in ways that are unpredictable from casual observation, we cannot hope to happen to measure-through sheer lucky guesswork-which environmental factors contribute importantly to the development of those characteristics; we first need to understand the mechanisms by which those traits develop.

5. Heritability doesn't necessarily have to do with biology

Environmental factors influence the development of highly heritable traits just as much as they influence the development of non-heritable traits (i.e., a trait like height, which is highly heritable in most developed nations, is very affected by environmental factors like diet). Likewise as can be seen from the example below, genetic factors influence the development of non-heritable traits just as much as they influence the development of highly heritable traits.

In fact, the least heritable features of human nature may be those that appear to be the most genetically determined. Consider the fact that having 5 fingers on each of our hands is not a particularly heritable characteristic (because most finger number variations in humans are attributable not to genetic variation, but to variations in experiences, such as accidents). Nonetheless, it is quite obvious that genetic factors play a role in determining the number of fingers we have on each of our hands!

6. Heritability says nothing about whether intelligence is more determined by genes or the environment

Because heritability is a population statistic, it has nothing to say about the individual. It makes no sense to ask whether a particular individual's intelligence has been more determined by nature or by nurture. As already stated, every trait develops through the interplay of genes and the environment. Nature and nurture are complementary, not at odds.

7. Twin Studies do not reveal the causes of intellectual development

Because adoption and twin studies that seek to account for trait variation in terms of genetic and environmental variation are always correlational, they reveal nothing about the causes of the appearance of the traits.

Adoption studies and twin studies do not entail the purposeful manipulation of either specific genes or specific environmental factors. Hence, such studies are unable to generate satisfying understandings of the factors and processes that contribute to the development of intelligence.

8. Heritability is not the same as heredity

Heritability does not tell us how likely it is that people's characteristics will be inherited by their children. Because traits that are 100% heritable can nonetheless be strongly influenced by environmental factors, it is not the case that a trait found to be heritable in a particular twin study will be passed from a given pair of parents to their children. Let's imagine that a study of alcoholism in the United States finds that the vast majority of the variation in people's tendencies to drink to excess can be accounted for by variation in their genes. If we then take a baby, newly born to a pair of alcoholic American parents, and raise it in a small village in southern India where it never encounters alcohol across its lifespan, it will not develop alcoholism. We often talk as if we "inherit" full-blown traits from our parents, like eye colors, nose shapes, and shyness. But all that we actually inherit from our parents are our genes and our genes' (and our) environments, factors that then construct full-blown traits during development. Consequently, it doesn't matter how heritable a trait is; if development of the offspring occurs in a different environment than the parent developed in, most bets are off.

Does heritability have any practicality?

We hope these eight facts have cleared up some misunderstandings. After reading these facts, it might be reasonable to ask, does the heritability coefficient have any practical value?

At the very least, heritability tells us how much of the variation in IQ can be accounted for by variation in genetic factors when development occurs in an exquisitely specific range of environments.

However, David S. Moore has recently argued that even this is not significant when we realize that the magnitude of any heritability statistic reflects the extent of variation in unidentified non-genetic factors that contribute to the development of the trait in question.

Because we cannot assess the variability (across our testing environments) of all the yet-to-be-identified non-genetic factors that influence IQ, Moore argues that estimates of the heritability of IQ are effectively un-interpretable and unable to be applied in any appropriate way. As Moore puts it in his journal article:

"Many psychologists continue to compute heritability statistics without questioning what exactly it is that they reveal to us. Unfortunately, careful consideration of these statistics suggests that they might not be applicable in any meaningful way, and so consequently, are uninteresting at best and misleading at worst."

Scott believes there are indeed some bits of information of importance that can be gleaned from the heritability coefficient, and lots of untapped potential for twin studies. More on that later.

© 2008 by Scott Barry Kaufman and David S. Moore.

What makes people who we are?

People are defined not only by their traits, preferences, interests, likes, and dislikes, but also by their friendships, social roles, family connections, and group memberships. The self is not just a “me,” but also a “we.”

How do people develop into who they are?

In reality, all biological and psychological characteristics are constructed during development, when genes interact with local environmental factors that can be influenced by the broader environment. Therefore, gene-environment interactions are understood to drive the development of all of our characteristics.

Is it biology or environment that causes us to be the person who we are?

Studies have concluded that human personalities and temperaments are shaped by both genetics and our environment; while we may be born with certain personality traits, there still is the possibility to develop others as we experience life.

Does nature or nurture determine your personality?

In fact, the major influence on personality is nonshared environmental influences, which include all the things that occur to us that make us unique individuals. These differences include variability in brain structure, nutrition, education, upbringing, and even interactions among the genes themselves.

Toplist

Última postagem

Tag